We are independent & ad-supported. We may earn a commission for purchases made through our links.
Advertiser Disclosure
Our website is an independent, advertising-supported platform. We provide our content free of charge to our readers, and to keep it that way, we rely on revenue generated through advertisements and affiliate partnerships. This means that when you click on certain links on our site and make a purchase, we may earn a commission. Learn more.
How We Make Money
We sustain our operations through affiliate commissions and advertising. If you click on an affiliate link and make a purchase, we may receive a commission from the merchant at no additional cost to you. We also display advertisements on our website, which help generate revenue to support our work and keep our content free for readers. Our editorial team operates independently of our advertising and affiliate partnerships to ensure that our content remains unbiased and focused on providing you with the best information and recommendations based on thorough research and honest evaluations. To remain transparent, we’ve provided a list of our current affiliate partners here.
Linguistics

Our Promise to you

Founded in 2002, our company has been a trusted resource for readers seeking informative and engaging content. Our dedication to quality remains unwavering—and will never change. We follow a strict editorial policy, ensuring that our content is authored by highly qualified professionals and edited by subject matter experts. This guarantees that everything we publish is objective, accurate, and trustworthy.

Over the years, we've refined our approach to cover a wide range of topics, providing readers with reliable and practical advice to enhance their knowledge and skills. That's why millions of readers turn to us each year. Join us in celebrating the joy of learning, guided by standards you can trust.

What Is the Input Hypothesis?

By Emily Daw
Updated: May 23, 2024
Views: 48,579
Share

The input hypothesis is a hypothesis in second language acquisition developed by Stephen Krashen, which states that a language learner gains the most benefit from receiving linguistic input that is just beyond his or her current interlanguage, or level of grammatical understanding. This type of input is known as comprehensible input or "i + 1," where "i" refers to the learner's interlanguage. According to Krashen, comprehensible input is most likely to be gained from interacting with another speaker of the language.

In some respects, the input hypothesis is fairly intuitive. Someone who understands only a few basic phrases of Chinese will not derive much benefit from listening to a scientific discourse in Chinese since it will be incomprehensible. Likewise, someone who is nearly fluent in Italian will not gain much grammatical knowledge from a child's picture book, because it will not introduce any new grammatical features.

Krashen, however, draws on more complex theories of second language acquisition to make his claim. The interlanguage hypothesis states that learners acquire the grammatical features of a language in a predictable order, and that at any given time the learner has an internally consistent grammatical framework known as the interlanguage. As the learner progresses, the interlanguage becomes increasingly similar to the target language's actual grammar. The input hypothesis states that input one stage closer to the target language — or i + 1 input — helps the learner acquire the next set of grammatical features. It is not enough, however, for a learner to receive comprehensible input passively; he or she must then analyze the new data in order to move the interlanguage forward.

The input hypothesis states that the best way for learners to gather comprehensible input is through a sort of trial-and-error process of communication. The learner seeks out conversation partners, who modify their speech until it becomes comprehensible to the learner. This process can be aided by nonverbal communication, such as by gestures and by feedback from the learner. When the process is successful, the learner's interlanguage grows to accommodate new grammatical features that he or she has observed.

Krashen claims that output, or speech production, has little to no relevance in second language acquisition. Many other researchers have criticized this position, however, saying that more advanced language learning and syntactic processing have to come through the trial-and-error process not only of comprehending, but also of speaking. Language output allows the speaker to test out his or her grammatical hypotheses and modify them if communication is not successful.

Share
Language & Humanities is dedicated to providing accurate and trustworthy information. We carefully select reputable sources and employ a rigorous fact-checking process to maintain the highest standards. To learn more about our commitment to accuracy, read our editorial process.
Discussion Comments
By jennythelib — On Apr 06, 2012

The idea of i + 1 makes a lot of sense for second language acquisition because it also jives with what we know about *first* language acquisition. Linguists, for instance, talk about "caretaker speech," which is not exactly baby talk - but caretakers talking to small children do tend to use simplified language that is just above the child's own current level.

The idea that output is not important seems less convincing. With children's first language development, research shows that it is important for adults communicating with the child to respond to the child's output. (If the child says "nana," for instance, you might say, "Do you want to eat a banana?")

Share
https://www.languagehumanities.org/what-is-the-input-hypothesis.htm
Copy this link
Language & Humanities, in your inbox

Our latest articles, guides, and more, delivered daily.

Language & Humanities, in your inbox

Our latest articles, guides, and more, delivered daily.